return

The Import and Export of Cognitive Science

Cognitive Science 30(6), 2006, forthcoming

 

Robert L. Goldstone

Indiana University

Department of Psychology, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN. 47405; rgoldsto@indiana.edu

 

Loet Leydesdorff

Amsterdam School of Communications Research (ASCoR), University of Amsterdam, Kloveniersburgwal 48, 1012 CX Amsterdam, The Netherlands; loet@leydesdorff.net; http://www.leydesdorff.net

 

 

From its inception, a large part of the motivation for Cognitive Science has been the need for an interdisciplinary journal for the study of minds and intelligent systems.  In the inaugural editorial for the journal, Allan Collins (1977) wrote “Current journals are fragmented along old disciplinary lines, so there is no common place for workers who approach these problems from different disciplines to talk to each other” (p. 1).  The interdisciplinarity of the journal has served a valuable cross-fertilization function for those who read the journal to discover articles written for and by practitioners across a wide range of fields.  The challenges of building and understanding intelligent systems are sufficiently large that they will most likely require the skills of psychologists, computer scientists, philosophers, educators, neuroscientists, and linguists collaborating and coordinating their efforts.

One threat to the interdisciplinarity of Cognitive Science, both the field and journal, is that it may become, or already be, too dominated by psychologists (Schunn, Crowley, & Okada, 1998; Von Eckardt, 2001).  One piece of evidence supporting this contention is that many of the manuscripts submitted to Cognitive Science are given “psychology” as field keyword by their authors.  In 2005,  psychology was a keyword for 51% of submissions, followed distantly by linguistics (17%), artificial intelligence (13%), neuroscience (10%), computer science (9%), and philosophy (8%) (these percentages sum to more than 100% because authors are not restricted to designating only a single field).

Another quantitative way to assess the interdisciplinarity of Cognitive Science as well as its general intellectual niche is to analyze aggregated journal-journal citations.  The Institute for Scientific Information (ISI) gathers data not only on how individual articles cite one another, but also on macroscopic citation patterns among journals.  Journals or sets of journals can be considered as proxies for fields.  As fields become established, they often create journals (Leydesdorff, Cozzens, & Van den Besselaar, 1994).   As Collins (1977) wrote when launching Cognitive Science, “In starting the journal we are just adding another trapping in the formation of a new discipline” (p. 1).  By studying the patterns of citations among journals that cite and are cited by Cognitive Science, we can better: 1) appreciate the scholarly ecology surrounding the journal and the journal’s role within this ecology, 2) establish competitor and alternate journals, and 3) determine the natural clustering of fields related to cognitive science (Leydesdorff, 2006; forthcoming).

 

Analyzing the Intellectual Ecology of Cognitive Science

The data we analyzed were the aggregated  journal-journal citation records of the 7379 journals in the 2004 Journal Citation Report, including both the Social Science Citation Index and the Science Citation Index.  The “Citing” data includes all of the journals (N=108) that were cited by articles appearing in Cognitive Science more than once in 2004.[1]  It thus represents the intellectual import to Cognitive ScienceThe “Cited” data includes all of the journals (N=180) that in 2004 had more than one citation to articles appearing in Cognitive Science. The “Cited” data can be interpreted as the intellectual export of Cognitive Science to other journals.  Square, asymmetrical matrices were created for both Cited and Citing data, with every cell containing the number of citations of one journal by the other.  The matrices were analyzed using factor analysis with a varimax rotation in SPSS. The factors can be interpreted as the major clusters of journals related to Cognitive Science.  We used the cosine instead of the Pearson correlation matrix for the visualization of citation patterns in Pajek (Salton & McGill, 1983; Ahlgren et al., 2003). The cosine is convenient for the visualization because this measure has no negative values and normalizes on the geometrical instead of the arithmetical mean[2].

 

Figure 1: The network of 107 journals cited by Cognitive Science in 2004.  The colors/shades of the nodes are provided by Pajek’s graph-based clustering algorithm.  The ellipses show clusters obtained by a factor analysis on the journal-to-journal citation export matrix.  Edges are only shown between journals that have a citation pattern with a cosine greater than 0.2, and the thickness of an edge reflects the citation proximity/cosine of the connected journals.  The specific journals can be identified by looking up their numbers in Table 1.  Only journals with ISI Impact Factors greater than 1.4 are assigned numbers.

 

       In terms of Cognitive Science’s import, in 2004 the journal cited 1090 articles appearing in 107 journals (one journal was dropped from the 108 journals cited by Cognitive Science because it was not cited by any other journal).  The Citing data, plotted in Figure 1, reveal the main categories of journals that import references to Cognitive Science.  A factor analysis revealed 5 prominent categories, and the impact of each of these categories is quantitatively measured by the proportion of variance in the citation matrix accounted for by each factor.  The most prominent journal category is cognition (proportion of variance = 14.7%), followed by neuroscience and general science (13.4%), then social and general psychology (8.7%), then development and education (5.9%), and finally artificial intelligence (5.9%). The values for each of the journals cited by Cognitive Science on each of the five factors is graphically displayed at http://www.cogsci.rpi.edu/CSJarchive/Supplemental/index.html .  We computed an import aggregate for each of these categories of journals by first classifying each journal according to the factor on which it has the greatest value, and then creating a cumulative count of the citations to these journal categories from Cognitive Science articles appearing in 2004 (see Cozzens & Leydesdorff, 1993 for the statistical construction of “macro-journals”).  The resulting import is: experimental psychology (540 citations), neuroscience and general science (328), social and general psychology (90), development and education (57), and artificial intelligence (75).  Figure 1 shows that Cognitive Science does not directly cite many education journals, but does cite journals that cite education journals.  The computer science and cognitive psychology journals have relatively strong within-category citations and relatively weak cross-category citations. 

 


Figure 2
: The network of 164 journals citing Cognitive Science in 2004. As with Figure 1, edges connecting journals reflect strong cross-citation patterns.  Journals with Impact Factors greater than 1.4 can be identified by looking up their numbers in Table 2.

 

   The Cited data, shown in Figure 2,  indicates that Cognitive Science is cited by more journals than it cites. It has 1113 citations in 2004, spread across the 180 journals.  A factor analysis yielded the following five factors in order of proportion variance accounted for: computer science/artificial intelligence (15.2%), cognition (7.6%), neuroscience and general science (5.0%), education (4.5%), and social/developmental psychology (3.8%).  We computed an export aggregate analogously to how we calculated the import, creating a cumulative count of the 2004 citations to Cognitive Science for each category.  The resulting export of citations is: computer science/artificial intelligence (429 citations), cognition (295), neuroscience and general science (130), education (172), and social/developmental psychology (87).

The two largest clusters of journals that cite Cognitive Science are cognitive psychology and computer science, with neuroscience rather densely interconnected with psychology.  Education, linguistics, and philosophy journals all have stronger presences in the Cited data than the Citing data networks.  Within the Cited network, Cognitive Science plays an important role in linking otherwise poorly related fields.  This is seen upon visual inspection of Figure 2 and can be measured in terms of betweenness centrality, which measures the number of shortest paths connecting journals that include Cognitive Science as a node (Freeman, 1977). Cognitive Science is on the shortest paths between nodes of the network in 30.3% of the cases, whereas the second-largest betweeness value is 5.1% (for Annual Review of Psychology) and the average betweeness values for the other cosines among citation patterns in Cognitive Science’s citation network is 0.61% (Leydesdorff, in preparation).  Thus, although Cognitive Science is cited highly in psychology, it has a strong export of citations to other fields, and it does satisfy at least one mission of an interdisciplinary journal – connecting together fields that might not otherwise efficiently exchange their knowledge.

   To provide a preliminary exploration of the evolution of Cognitive Science’s export over time, we analyzed citations to Cognitive Science in the 1988 ISI database. A factor analysis revealed the following five factors in order of proportion variance accounted for: cognition (6.9%), artificial intelligence (6.0%), education (4.0%), developmental and social psychology (3.2%), and human-computer interaction (3.2%).  Cognitive Science articles were cited 442 times in 1988, broken down by the five field factors as follows: cognition (101 citations), artificial intelligence (111), education (80), developmental and social psychology (56), and human-computer interaction (94).

 

Discussion

From these data, we draw three conclusions about the role of Cognitive Science in cognitive science, and about the structure of cognitive science more broadly.  First, within the journal ecology of Cognitive Science, the following fields robustly emerge: 1) computer science combined with artificial intelligence, 2) cognition, 3) neuroscience (combined with general science), 4) developmental/social/educational psychology (with developmental psychology sometimes cohering more tightly with social psychology, and sometimes with education).  Although these fields reliably fall out of the factor analyses, a closer examination reveals that human-computer interaction played a larger role in Cognitive Science in 1988 than 2004.  Conversely, neuroscience has a considerably greater influence on Cognitive Science in 2004 than it did in 1988. Journals related to language show up in several different categories within Cognitive Science’s journal ecology.  Philosophy and linguistics, although they are core disciplines to cognitive science, have relatively small roles to play in the journal ecology of Cognitive Science.  This is, in part, because not all philosophy and linguistics journals are included in ISI’s general or social science indexes.  However, Cognitive Science is more weakly connected to linguistics and philosophy journal that are indexed than it is to computer science, psychology or neuroscience journals.

Second, the specifics of the claim for the interdisciplinary relevance of Cognitive Science depend upon whether we refer to export or import.  Cognitive Science is heavily cited by computer science and artificial intelligence journals, but does not cite these journals as much.  In terms of import to Cognitive Science, the four most influential journal categories all relate to psychology.  Note that the asymmetry in citations to and from computer science journals cannot be explained by computer science articles having fewer references than social science articles; Cognitive Science was cited 429 times by computer science articles, but only cited 75 computer science articles.  Asymmetries in import and export factors are common in science (Boyack, Klavans, & Börner, 2005), and are not necessarily ominous signs (e.g. business management journals cite psychology journals far more than vice versa). Cognitive Science’s import profile can be interpreted as suggesting that the journal is at some risk of being psychology-centric in the literature its articles use to build their arguments.  Its export profile, however, suggests that the journal functions as a window for computer scientists on new developments in psychology and relevant fields in the social sciences.

Third, the claim for interdisciplinary importance is far stronger when considering betweeness.  If Cognitive Science did not exist, the citation path connecting journals would be far longer.  Admittedly, this analysis is predicated upon selecting journals that cite, or are cited by, Cognitive Science.  Still, Cognitive Science plays a unique bridging role in efficiently transferring information across psychology, computer science, neuroscience, and education.  In addition to gauging interdisciplinarity by measuring the number of articles with a multidisciplinary team (Schunn et al., 1998) or the number of disciplines that actively engage in cognitive science research (Von Eckardt, 2001), the betweeness measure offers yet another perspective on what it means for cognitive science to be interdisciplinary -- to play a critical role in connecting fields.  Similar to how social networks benefit greatly by having individuals span and therefore connect otherwise disjoined cliques (Granovetter, 1973), the scientific network benefits from having intellectual communities that effectively merge perspectives, tools, and methods.

More generally, we are optimistic about the possibilities for applying cognitive science and scientometric analyses to understanding the structure and evolution of cognitive science itself.  Even more broadly speaking, science is a web of interrelated fields, and interdisciplinary research stitches together scientific communities that are constantly under pressure of increasing fractionation.  Counteracting the trend toward ever increasing specialization in science, interdisciplinary approaches promote meaning-building by creating rich, interconnected networks of knowledge. In much the same way that a concept or belief does not mean anything in isolation but rather only within a network of other concepts and beliefs (Quine & Ullian, 1970; Stich, 1983), the meaning of a scientific result is best gauged by measuring its specific impact on the entire knowledge network. Our techniques reveal these local citation impacts and to understand the mechanisms. Interdisciplinary fields such as cognitive science have wisely placed their bets on creating meaning not only by decomposing knowledge into smaller and smaller nuggets, but also by interrelating and connecting these nuggets.

               


References

 

 

Ahlgren, P., Jarneving, B., & Rousseau, R. (2003). Requirement for a cocitation similarity measure, with special reference to Pearson's correlation coefficient. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 54(6), 550-560.

Boyack, K. W., Klavans, R., & Börner, K. (2005).  Mapping the backbone of science.  Scientometrics, 64, 351-374.

Collins, A. (1977).  Why cognitive science.  Cognitive Science, 1, 1-2.

Cozzens, S. E., & Leydesdorff, L. (1993). Journal Systems as Macro-Indicators of Structural Change in the Sciences. In A. F. J. van Raan, R. E. de Bruin, H. F. Moed, A. J. Nederhof & R. W. J. Tijssen (Eds.), Science and Technology in a Policy Context (pp. 219-233). Leiden: DSWO/Leiden University.

Freeman, L. C. (1977). A set of measures of centrality based on betweenness. Sociometry, 40(1), 35-41.

Granovetter, M. S. (1973).  The strength of weak ties.  American Journal of Sociology, 78, 1360-1380.

Leydesdorff, L. (2006). Can scientific journals be classified in terms of aggregated journal-journal citation relations using the journal citation reports? Journal of the American Society for Information Science & Technology, 57(5), 601-613.

Leydesdorff, L. (forthcoming).  Visualization of the Citation Impact Environments of Scientific Journals: An online mapping exercise, Journal of the American Society of Information Science and Technology, (in press).

Leydesdorff, L. (in preparation). “Betweenness” as an Indicator of the Interdisciplinarity of Scientific Journals. Paper to be presented at the 9th International S&T Indicators Conference. Leuven (Belgium), September 7-9, 2006.

Leydesdorff, L., Cozzens, S. E., & Van den Besselaar, P. (1994). Tracking areas of strategic importance using scientometric journal mappings. Research Policy, 23, 217-229.

Quine, W. V., & Ullian, J. S. (1970).  The Web of Belief.  New York: McGraw-Hill.

Salton, G., & McGill, M. J. (1983). Introduction to Modern Information Retrieval. Auckland, etc.:McGraw-Hill.

Rogers, Y., Scaife, M., & Rizzo, A. (2005).  Interdisciplinarity: An emergent or engineered process?  In S. Derry, C.  D. Schunn, and M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.)  Interdisciplinary collaboration: An emerging cognitive science.  Lawrence Erlbaum & Associates (pp. 265-286).

Schunn, C., Crowley, K., & Okada, T. (1998).  The growth of multidisciplinarity in the Cognitive Science Society.  Cognitive Science, 22, 107-130.

Stich, S. P. (1983).  From Folk Psychology to Cognitive Science: The Case Against Belief.  Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.

Von Eckardt, B. (2001).  Multidisciplinarity and cognitive science.  Cognitive Science, 25, 453-570.

 


Author Notes

 

We thank Robin Kramer for extensive work on preparing the figures and tables, and Katy Börner for helpful comments.  This research was funded by National Science Foundation grant 052792, and Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences grant R305H050116.  Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to rgoldsto@indiana.edu or Robert Goldstone, Psychology Department, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana 47405.  Further information about the laboratory of the first author can be found at http://cognitrn.psych.indiana.edu/.  For further information about the supporting software, visit http://www.leydesdorff.net/jcr04, and for further reading about these techniques, visit http://www.leydesdorff.net/list.htm.

 


Table 1: Journals that are cited by Cognitive Science, ordered by their factor of maximum value.

 

Journal Number

Journal Name

Journal Number

Journal Name

Journal Number

Journal Name

1

Cognitive Science

26

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

51

Psychological Bulletin

2

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

27

Current Biology

52

Annual Review of Psychology

3

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A - Human Experimental Psychology

28

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

53

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

4

Cognitive Psychology

29

Cognitive Brain Research

54

Psychological Science

5

Journal of Experimental Psychology - General

30

Science

55

Current Directions in Psychological Science

6

Memory & Cognition

31

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B - Biological Sciences

56

American Psychologist

7

Journal of Experimental Psychology - Learning Memory and Cognition

32

Nature

57

Human-Computer Interaction

8

Psychology of Learning and Motivation - Advances in Research and Theory

33

Journal of Neurophysiology

58

Cognitive Development

9

Journal of Memory and Language

34

Human Brain Mapping

59

Child Development

10

Psychological Review

35

Neural Computation

60

Developmental Psychology

11

Acta Psychologica

36

Visual Neuroscience

61

Developmental Science

12

Journal of Experimental Psychology - Human Perception and Performance

37

Neuroimage

62

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

13

Visual Cognition

38

Journal of Physiology - London

63

Journal of Educational Psychology

14

Perception & Psychophysics

39

Brain

64

Journal of the Optical Society of America A - Optics Image Science and Vision

15

Cognition

40

Neuropsychologia

65

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America

16

Brain and Language

41

Behavioral and Brain Sciences

66

Computational Intelligence

17

Cognitive Neuropsychology

42

Experimental Brain Research

67

Artificial Intelligence

18

Nature Reviews Neuroscience

43

Vision Research

68

IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation

19

Annual Review of Neuroscience

44

Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science

69

Machine Learning

20

Nature Neuroscience

45

IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering

70

Computational Linguistics

21

Neuroreport

46

Neurology

71

IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence

22

Neuron

47

Journal of the Learning Sciences

72

Journal of Experimental Psychology - Animal Behavior Processes

23

Cerebral Cortex

48

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

73

Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior

24

Journal of Neuroscience

49

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

 

 

25

Trends in Cognitive Sciences

50

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

 

 



Table 2:  Journals Citing Cognitive Science, ordered by their factor of maximum value.

 

Journal Number

Journal Name

Journal Number

Journal Name

Journal Number

Journal Name

1

Cognitive Science

25

Journal of Experimental Psychology - Human Perception and Performance

49

Cognitive Neuropsychology

2

IEEE Intelligent Systems

26

Memory

50

Journal of Neurophysiology

3

Autonomous Agents and Multi-Agent Systems

27

Acta Psychologica

51

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

4

Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research

28

Cognition

52

Nature

5

Artificial Intelligence

29

Visual Cognition

53

Journal of Motor Behavior

6

Computational Intelligence

30

Behavioral and Brain Sciences

54

Vision Research

7

IEEE Transactions on Software Engineering

31

Psychology and Aging

55

Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B - Biological Sciences

8

User Modeling and User-Adapted Interaction

32

Consciousness and Cognition

56

Hearing Research

9

IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks

33

Journal of Experimental Psychology - Applied

57

Journal of Applied Psychology

10

Computational Linguistics

34

Academic Medicine

58

Review of Educational Research

11

Neural Networks

35

Medical Education

59

Educational Psychologist

12

Neural Computation

36

Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology

60

Journal of Educational Psychology

13

Adaptive Behavior

37

Neuropsychologia

61

Learning and Instruction

14

Annual Review of Information Science and Technology

38

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

62

Journal of the Learning Sciences

15

Journal of Experimental Psychology - General

39

Brain

63

Leadership Quarterly

16

Memory & Cognition

40

Cognitive Brain Research

64

Cognitive Development

17

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

41

Neuroreport

65

Developmental Psychology

18

Journal of Experimental Psychology - Learning Memory and Cognition

42

Trends in Cognitive Sciences

66

Child Development

19

Cognitive Psychology

43

Cortex

67

Developmental Science

20

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A - Human Experimental Psychology

44

Trends in Neurosciences

68

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

21

Psychological Review

45

Neuroimage

69

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines

22

Psychology of Learning and Motivation - Advances in Research and Theory

46

Brain and Language

70

Annual Review of Psychology

23

Journal of Memory and Language

47

Journal of Neurolinguistics

71

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

24

Psychological Science

48

Experimental Brain Research

72

Advances in Experimental Social Psychology


 

return

 


[1] Single citations among journals are aggregated by the ISI under the category “All others”.

[2] The cosine matrices based on the citation patterns of the ISI journal set are brought online at http://www.leydesdorff.net/jcr04/cited and http://www.leydesdorff.net/jcr04/citing, respectively. Pajek can be retrieved at http://vlado.fmf.uni-lj.si/pub/networks/pajek/ .